Why are some people left-handed? - Daniel M. Abrams

404

If you know an older left-handed person,

chances are they had to learn to write or eat with their right hand.

And in many parts of the world,

it's still common practice to force children to use their "proper" hand.

Even the word for right also means correct or good,

not just in English, but many other languages, too.

But if being left-handed is so wrong,

then why does it happen in the first place?

Today, about 1/10 of the world's population are left-handed.

Archeological evidence shows that it's been that way

for as long as 500,000 years,

with about 10% of human remains

showing the associated differences in arm length and bone density,

and some ancient tools and artifacts showing evidence of left-hand use.

And despite what many may think, handedness is not a choice.

It can be predicted even before birth based on the fetus' position in the womb.

So, if handedness is inborn, does that mean it's genetic?

Well, yes and no.

Identical twins, who have the same genes, can have different dominant hands.

In fact, this happens as often as it does with any other sibling pair.

But the chances of being right or left-handed

are determined by the handedness of your parents

in surprisingly consistent ratios.

If your father was left-handed but your mother was right-handed,

you have a 17% chance of being born left-handed,

while two righties will have a left-handed child only 10% of the time.

Handedness seems to be determined by a roll of the dice,

but the odds are set by your genes.

All of this implies there's a reason

that evolution has produced this small proportion of lefties,

and maintained it over the course of millennia.

And while there have been several theories

attempting to explain why handedness exists in the first place,

or why most people are right-handed,

a recent mathematical model

suggests that the actual ratio reflects a balance

between competitive and cooperative pressures on human evolution.

The benefits of being left-handed

are clearest in activities involving an opponent,

like combat or competitive sports.

For example, about 50% of top hitters in baseball have been left-handed.

Why?

Think of it as a surprise advantage.

Because lefties are a minority to begin with,

both right-handed and left-handed competitors

will spend most of their time encountering

and practicing against righties.

So when the two face each other,

the left-hander will be better prepared against this right-handed opponent,

while the righty will be thrown off.

This fighting hypothesis,

where an imbalance in the population

results in an advantage for left-handed fighters or athletes,

is an example of negative frequency-dependent selection.

But according to the principles of evolution,

groups that have a relative advantage

tend to grow until that advantage disappears.

If people were only fighting and competing throughout human evolution,

natural selection would lead to more lefties being the ones that made it

until there were so many of them,

that it was no longer a rare asset.

So in a purely competitive world,

50% of the population would be left-handed.

But human evolution has been shaped by cooperation, as well as competition.

And cooperative pressure

pushes handedness distribution in the opposite direction.

In golf, where performance doesn't depend on the opponent,

only 4% of top players are left-handed,

an example of the wider phenomenon of tool sharing.

Just as young potential golfers

can more easily find a set of right-handed clubs,

many of the important instruments that have shaped society

were designed for the right-handed majority.

Because lefties are worse at using these tools,

and suffer from higher accident rates,

they would be less successful in a purely cooperative world,

eventually disappearing from the population.

So by correctly predicting the distribution

of left-handed people in the general population,

as well as matching data from various sports,

the model indicates

that the persistence of lefties as a small but stable minority

reflects an equilibrium

that comes from competitive and cooperative effects

playing out simultaneously over time.

And the most intriguing thing

is what the numbers can tell us about various populations.

From the skewed distribution of pawedness in cooperative animals,

to the slightly larger percentage of lefties

in competitive hunter-gatherer societies,

we may even find that the answers to some puzzles of early human evolution

are already in our hands.